/ Mechanisms
Regulatory Mechanisms
in Biosystems
ISSN 2519-8521 (Print) ISSN 2520-2588 (Online) Regul. Mech. Biosyst., 2020, 11(3), 354-359 doi: 10.15421/022054
The effect of miR-101 on the memory of rats with a model of Alzheimer's disease
O. G. Berchenko, N. O. Levicheva, D. O. Bevzyuk, V. V. Sokolik
Institute ofNeurology, Psychiatry and Narcology, Kharkiv, Ukraine
Received 15.07.2020 Received in revisedform
23.08.2020 Accepted 25.08.2020
Institute ofNeurology,
Academy Pavlov st., 46 Kharkov, 61068, Ukraine. Tel.: +38-095-055-38-80. E-mail:
berchenko. olga@ukr. net
Berchenko, O. G., Levicheva, N. O., Bevzyuk, D. O., & Sokolik, V. V. (2020). The effect of miR-101 on the memory of rats with a model of Alzheimer's disease. Regulatory Mechanisms in Biosystems, 11(3), 354-359. doi:10.15421/022054
Memory impairment is a hallmark of Alzheimer's disease. The clinical diagnosis of the disease is made in the later stages of its development, when specific therapy of the disease is not always effective. Therefore, the detection of early behavioral manifestations of memory disorders in the development of the disease will allow the use of preventive therapy aimed at stopping the death of neurons in brain structures. A neuroethological study of working, spatial, and emotional memory was performed in rats 15-16 months of age with a model of early manifestations of Alzheimer's disease induced by stereotactic administration of ff-amyloid peptide 40 aggregates into the hippocampus. Changes in the neuroethological components of working and spatial memory have been identified. Testing of working memory showed a violation in rats of recognizing the shape of identical objects, reducing experimental activity to unfamiliar objects and their differentiation. Spatial orientation disorders have been identified in the Barnes labyrinth. Emotional memory research has shown the preservation of innate forms of protective adaptive behaviour. At the same time, vegetative indicators reflected an increase in emotional tension. Intranasal administration of liposomal miRNA miR-101 involved in liposomes to rats with a model of early manifestations of Alzheimer's disease improved neuroethological parameters of working and spatial memory. Restoration of the level of research activity and differentiation of familiar and unfamiliar objects in the testing of working memory in rats has been established. Spatial memory in Barnes labyrinth testing was improved by reproducing spatial orientation skills and relieving emotional stress. Thus, the intranasal use of miR-101 in Alzheimer's disease is a promising approach to prevent the development of amyloidosis and preserve memory in the early manifestations of Alz-heimer's disease.
Keywords: spatial; working; emotional memory; hippocampus; neurodegeneration.
Introduction
One of the first symptoms of Alzheimer's disease is the irreversible deterioration of memory up to dementia. In patients, Alzheimer's dementia is distinguished, which is due to the occurrence and course of cerebral amyloidosis (formation of a local excess of ff-amyloid peptide (Af) aggregates in the foci of cholinergic neurons of the neocortex and hippocampus). The presence of a long asymptomatic period is a feature of neu-rodegenerative pathology of Alzheimer's disease. During this period, the manifestations of specific disorders of memory and cognitive functions are compensated by the mechanisms of the brain (Purdenko, 2014). It has been shown that the neocortex and hippocampus (Insel & Takehara-Nishiuchi, 2013; Huijgen & Samson, 2015) are structures that provide mechanisms for the formation of different types of memory: emotional, working, spatial (Silachjov et al., 2008; Antunes & Biala, 2012; Grigor-chuk, 2013). In patients with Alzheimer's disease, it is the neurons of the neocortex and hippocampus that suffer the most from the toxicity of Af oligomers, which subsequently form senile plaques characteristic of amy-loidogenic pathology (Mukhin, 2013). Increased forgetting of available information as an early manifestation of the disease is due to a decrease in the amount and time of information retention in working memory. Violation of the mechanisms of consolidation of memory traces is observed already in the clinical stage of Alzheimer's disease.
Currently, the clinical treatment protocols for Alzheimer's disease prefer symptomatic treatment with central acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, cholinomimetics and NMDA receptor antagonists in compensatory therapy, as well as neuroprotection with nootropics, which aims to preserve and maintain life of neurons (Bullock, 2002). However, such a strategy is not able to prevent the constantly progressing dementia, but only to delay it. Therefore, one of the leading strategies for the treatment of Alzheimer's
disease is the development of approaches that do not affect the symptoms, but the pathological mechanisms of cerebral amyloidosis. Individual y-secretase modulators (an enzyme that secretes ff-amyloid peptides from Af precursor protein (AfPP) during amyloidogenic processing) have been shown to be able to specifically inhibit its activity against AfPP without interfering with the metabolism of other substrates of this enzyme (Raven et al., 2017). It has been experimentally found that anti-inflammatory therapy, such as curcumin, has a positive effect not only on chronic inflammation, which invariably accompanies the course of amyloidosis, but also directly inhibits the excessive formation of ff-amyloid peptides (Sokolik et al., 2017).
Gene therapy that regulates or blocks overexpression of a single gene at the posttranscriptional level by means of the corresponding miRNA has proved to be promising (Wang et al., 2012; Shaik et al., 2018). It is shown that miR-101 is able to specifically interact with its "seed" site with the template RNA of the Af precursor protein and, thus, repress the expression of the AfPP gene (Francis et al., 2010). However, since their discovery in 1998, miRNAs have not lived up to the expectations of pharmaceutical companies (Eisenstein, 2019), because the problem of targeted delivery of these molecules to target organs and body tissues has not been solved.
Upon introduction into the bloodstream or subcutaneously, the bulk of the miRNA ended up in the liver regardless of the type of carrier, whether liposomes, liproteins, N-acetylgalactosamine (GalNAc), etc. To solve the problem of direct delivery of miR-101 to the central nervous system, in addition to direct traumatic injections into the cerebrospinal fluid, you can use its intranasal course administration in liposomal form.
The aim of the study is to explore the influence intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101 on memory processes in late adult rats in experimental modeling of Alzheimer's disease.
Materials and methods
Research design. Procedures with experimental animals were approved by the Commission on Ethics and Deontology of SI "Institute of Neurology, Psychiatry and Addiction of the National Academy of Medical Sciences of Ukraine" and performed in accordance with the General Ethical Principles of Animal Experiments (Kyiv, 2011), 'The procedure for conducting experiments and experiments on animals by scientific institutions" (No 249 of 01.03.2012), the Law of Ukraine "On protection of animals from cruel treatment" (№ 3447-IV of21.02.2006). The study was conducted on a general group of male rats 15-16 months of age, weighing 300-500 g (n = 33). Animals were randomly divided into two groups: experimental (n = 17) and comparison group (n = 16).
Simulation of Alzheimer's disease in all rats was performed by stereotactic administration of aggregated Ap40_Human (China Peptides Co, China), in the left hippocampus at a dose of 15 nmol/L P-amyloid peptide 40, which was pre-dissolved in double-distilled water and aggregate during the day (Fig. 1). Large coarse conglomerates of Ap40 were dispersed by ultrasound and sterilized immediately before administration. The volume of the injected suspension of AP40 in aggregate form was 10 pL, the rate of injection through the needle of the chromatographic syringe -0.03 pL/s, the duration of administration - 5 minutes. Stereotactic coordinates ofthe hippocampus were determined from brain maps of rats Fifko-va and Marshall, as quoted by Buresh et al. (1962), which corresponded to the distance from the point of intersection of the sagittal suture with bregma: F = 2 mm, L = 2 mm, H = 3.5 mm. The operations were performed under general anesthesia under intra-abdominal injection of sodium thi-opental solution (50 mg/kg).
Fig. 1. Stereotactic administration of P-amyloid peptide 40 aggregates in the left hippocampus of rats
To study the processes of memory recovery in animals with a model of Alzheimer's disease we performed intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101 (OOO "NPF Sintal", RF) to rats of the experimental group for 10 days. The liposomal form of miR-101 was obtained by the method of lipid films (Shulga, 2014). For 10 sessions of administration, each animal of the experimental group received 2.5 x 1014 molecules of miR-101, the volume of a single portion of liposomal miRNA was 20 pL. The effect of miR-101 was chosen because it is a key operator of matrix RNA (mRNA) function for the AP precursor protein (APPP) due to the formation of a complex with its subsequent deactivation and therefore able to inhibit APPP synthesis and amyloidogenic processing of the latter (Vi-lardo et al., 2010; Amakiri et al., 2019). Animals in the reference group did not receive liposomal miR-101.
Neuroethological testing of working memory. Working memory was characterized based on the natural ability of rats to learn something new (reaction to the novelty of the stimulus). We evaluated the ability of animals to recognize new objects based on the detection of changes in the physical properties of the object and the fixation of these characteristics
(memorization) (Abdurasulova et al., 2019). The test of working memory was as follows. Two days before testing, the rat was placed in an experimental cage for 10 min to adapt to the experimental conditions. On the 3rd day, the animal was placed in the center of the cage in front of two identical objects A1 and A2 (identical wooden cubes measuring 2.0 x 4.5 x 3.0 cm; located at a distance of 15 cm from each other) and for 5 min we recorded the duration of the research reaction: the total time of sniffing objects at a distance of less than 2 cm or touching them with the nose/foot (training phase). On day 4, a similar procedure was performed (test phase), with object A2 being replaced by any new object B (B1, B2, B3 or B4). Namely: object B1 - a rectangular parallelepiped measuring 1.0 x 8.0 x 1.5 cm, object B2 - a triangular pyramid measuring 4.0 x 5.0 cm, object B3 - a sphere with a diameter of 5 cm, object B4 - a truncated cone measuring 3.5 x 5.0 cm. We recorded the study time of the new object (B1, B2, B3 or B4) and the familiar object A1, and calculated the percentage of discrimination, which was the time spent on the study of the new object, relative to the total study time of both objects in the testing phase (Tzyy-Nan & Yi-Ping, 2014; Gulljaev et al., 2017). Working memory metrics was determined in the initial state, on the 5-6th day after intrahypocampal administration of Ap40 aggregates, on the 8-9th day of intranasal course of liposomal miR-101.
Exploration of spatial (short-term and long-term) memory in the Barnes labyrinth. A Barnes' labyrinth was used to study spatial memory (Fig. 2). At the heart of the Barnes maze technique is the physiologically determined desire of rats to avoid brightly lit open spaces and the search for a protective chamber with a focus on the configuration of distal visual signals located around the test area (Attar et al., 2013; Sysoev, 2019). The Barnes labyrinth includes a round platform (121 cm in diameter) with 16 evenly spaced holes (9 cm in diameter) that is raised 85 cm above the floor. The holes are 7.5 cm from the edge of the platform. Under one of the holes is a protective chamber - a plastic box measuring 24 x 15 cm. The surface ofthe platform is equipped with a bright light source (150 W), which is at a height of40 cm from the middle of the maze. Orientations of animals in space served as visual signals - large geometric shapes located on both sides of the labyrinth. The starting area for the experimental animal was a cylinder with a diameter of 20 cm.
Fig. 2. Barnes labyrinth for the study of spatial memory in experimental animals
In the Barnes labyrinth, the animal went through three stages: adaptation, training and testing of short-term and long-term memory. During adaptation, the animal was placed in a cylinder located in the center of the platform and the light was turned on. After 10 minutes, the cylinder was lifted and the animal was directed to the protective chamber. If the rat did not enter the protective chamber for 2 min, it was forcibly directed to the hole with the protective chamber. As soon as the animal was in the protective chamber, the light was turned off and the animal was left inside the box for another 2 minutes. During training, the animal was also placed in a cylinder in the starting area and the light turned on. After 10 minutes, the cylinder was raised and the animal was given the opportunity to see the maze for 3 minutes. In the Barnes labyrinth we recorded the time of successful search for a hole with a protective chamber and the trajectory of movement. The study was stopped after the rat entered the protective chamber, in which the animal was left for another 1 minute. We conducted 4 studies per day with an interval between studies of 15 min for 4 days. Testing of short-term memory was performed on the 5th day with a closed hole in the protective chamber, 24 hours after the last day of training. The procedure was performed similarly to the training process, but the animal was allowed to explore the maze only for 90 seconds.
We determined whether the rat remembered the location of the protective chamber or not. The time of finding the protective chamber, the number of approaches to it, the time spent near the protective chamber, the trajectory of the rat, autonomic reactions were recorded. The same testing was repeated on the 11th day after intrahypocampal administration of Ap40 aggregates (Alzheimer's disease model) and after the 10th day of intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101 (in the experimental group) and on the 26th day of the Alzheimer's disease model without treatment (in the comparison group). To do this, rats were retrained to search for a protective chamber, but located in a different location than in previous tests.
Evaluation of long-term spatial memory was performed similarly to short-term, only without any prior training. All parameters in experimental animals were recorded in the initial state, on the 7th and 21st day after intrahypocampal administration of Ap40 aggregates and after a course (10 days) of intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101.
Testing of emotional memory by the method of conditioned-reflex emotional reaction of avoidance. The study of the features of emotional memory was performed using the technique of conditioned-reflex emotional response avoidance (CRERA) (Bevziuk et al, 2020). The emotional conditioned reaction of avoidance was performed in a specially equipped chamber divided into two parts by a partition with a window. Voltage was applied to the floor electrodes in this chamber (threshold of nociceptive pacing stimulation from 3.0 to 3.5 V); the current was turned on by closing the contacts. The conditional signal for the rat was the sound of a metronome with a frequency of 120 min-1, and its unconditional reinforcement was carried out by electric current. The animals in the chamber were given a conditional signal, after which the next 10 s of sound was combined with unconditional reinforcement. If during this time the rat did not move to another part of the chamber, the metronome was turned off, and electric stimulation was carried out for another 5 s. The duration of the inter-signal period was chosen randomly in the range of 2 to 3 minutes to prevent the development of a conditioned reflex in the animal for a while. They were presented with no more than 6 combinations of conditional signal and unconditional reinforcement per day. The conditioned reflex was considered to be produced if the rat moved to another part of the chamber during one experimental day during the action of the conditioned signal at least 5 times in a row. The share of such behavioral responses as extrasignal, signaling, avoidance responses, sample responses, and auto-nomic responses that accompany CRERA production was expressed as a percentage of the number of conditional signal combinations with unconditional reinforcement presented at individual stages of the experiment on average. Assessment of memory function was performed before the introduction of Ap40 aggregates, on the 5th day of Alzheimer's disease simulation and after 10 days of intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101 or without treatment.
Statistical data processing was performed using the program Statistica 6.0 (Statsoft Inc., USA, 2001). After pre-establishing the normality of the data distribution in each group, the mean and standard deviation (x ± SD) were calculated. Single-factor dispersion analysis ANOVA was used to detect statistically significant differences between groups. Differences were considered significant at P < 0.05 using the Tukey test.
Results
Working memory. Administration to rats of aggregated P-amyloid peptide 40 in the hippocampus led to a gradual violation of the mechanisms ofworking memory. On the 5th day after administration of the neurotoxin, the rats' information about the experience of recognizing familiar objects was stored. The study time of the same familiar subjects did not differ from the original data. However, the study time of the new unfamiliar stimulus B2 (triangular pyramid) decreased compared to baseline (Table 1).
On the 16th day after administration of the neurotoxin, there was a deterioration in the recognition of identical objects. The time of their study was significantly (P < 0.05) different from the original data. Interest in the unfamiliar object B3 (bullet) decreased. The study time was significantly (P < 0.05) decreased compared to baseline and values after the 5th day of neurotoxin administration (Table 1). The discrimination rate was 53% vs. 65% of baseline values (Fig. 3).
Table 1
Time of study of familiar and unfamiliar objects in rats after intrahypocampal administration of P-amyloid peptide 40 aggregates and intranasal administration ofliposomal miR-101 (s, x ± SD)
Characteristics of the group
Output data
Model of Alzheimer's disease
The model of the disease Alzheimer's without treatment
Intranasal liposomal administration miR-101in
Alzheimer's disease
Date 0th day 5-6th day 16-17th days 16-17th days
Group name common common comparison experimental
n 33 33 16 17
Identical A1 20.2 ± 2.5 10.0 ± 2.0s 12.8 ± 2.5s 8.2 ± 2.8a
objects A2 21.6 ± 2.3 9.8 ± 1.7s 22.7 ± 4.8b 8.9 ± 2.6a c
Familiar and unfamiliar objects A1 B1 9.6 ± 1.7 23.5 ± 3.1d 10.1 ± 1.3 12.3 ± 4.5 13.2 ± 1.7
B2 B3 B4 - 16.2 ± 0.8s" d 11.2 ± 1.8ab 29.8 ± 5.1b, c, d
Note: a - P < 0.05 compared to baseline, b - P < 0.05 compared to the 5-6th day of Alzheimer's disease, c - P < 0.05 compared to the 16-17th day of Alzheimer's disease without treatment, d - P < 0.05 compared to the familiar object (A1); the reliability of the differences between the samples was evaluated using the single-factor dispersion analysis (ANOVA), using the Tukey test; A1 and A2 - familiar objects, B1, B2, B3, B4 - unfamiliar objects; "-" - not investigated.
portion, % 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0
I
II
III
IV
Fig. 3. Coefficient of discrimination in the test working memory in rats under the influence of intrahypocampal administration of aggregates of P-amyloid peptide 40 and intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101: I - baseline, II - model of Alzheimer's disease on the 6th day ofthe study, III - model of Alzheimer's disease on the 17th day ofthe study without treatment, IV - intranasal administration ofliposomal miR-101 in Alzheimer' s disease on the 17th day ofthe study; x ± SD, n = 33; a - P < 0.05 compared to the original data; b - P < 0.05 compared with the 17th day of Alzheimer's disease; here and in Figure 6 the reliability of the differences between the samples was evaluated using ANOVA
Intranasal administration of miR-101 in the form of liposomes reproduced the perception of new information on the study of identical objects and the differentiation of different shaped objects. Rats spent the same amount oftime exploring similarly shaped objects. Objects that differed in shape aroused the interest of animals. The experimental activity significantly (P < 0.05) increased compared with the indicators observed in the simulation ofAlzheimer's disease (Table 1, Fig. 3).
Spatial memory. A study of short-term spatial memory in rats in the Barnes labyrinth showed that the introduction of Ap40 aggregates reduced the time spent at the opening of the protective chamber on both the 11th and 26th day ofAlzheimer's disease simulation (P < 0.05, Table 2), i. e. in animals the forgetting of the location of the opening of the protective chamber was observed.
It should be noted that for rats under the conditions of testing, the entrance to the protective chamber was closed, which increased their search activity. There was a movement of rats in a circle with a consecutive longer study of false holes (Fig. 4). That is, at an early stage of Alzheimer's disease there was an inhibition of the process of extracting spatial information from the engram of memory and a partial violation of its reproduction Intranasal course of liposomal miR-101 for 10 days in rats with an
b
experimental model of Alzheimer's disease restored short-term spatial memory. A probable decrease in the search time for the protective chamber was revealed (Table 2). The animals immediately went to the protective chamber, examined the entrance to it, and the trajectory was similar to the original data, which indicated an improvement in the analytical processes of the rat brain under the influence of miR-101 (Fig. 4d).
Table 2
Influence of intrahypocampal administration of ff-amyloid
peptide 40 aggregates and intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101
on short-term spatial memory in rats in the Barnes labyrinth (x ± SD)
(Fig. 5c). There was a probable decrease in the number of approaches to the security chamber. Intranasal administration to rats of the experimental group of liposomal miR-101 for 10 days restored memories of the location of the protective chamber. Animals of the experimental group spent a large amount of time near it, compared with rats of the comparison group (Fig. 5c, d).
Table 3
Effect of intrahypocampal administration off-amyloid peptide 40 aggregates and intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101 on long-term spatial memory in rats in the Barnes labyrinth (x ± SD)
Characteristics of 1he group Output data Model of Alzheimer's disease The model oflhe Intranasal liposomal disease administration Alzheimer's miR-101 in without treatment Alzheimer's disease Characteristics of the group Output data „, , , „ The model ofthe Intranasal liposomal A5Z2fs disease Alz- administration disease heimer's without miR-101 in treatment Alzheimer's disease
Date 0th day 11th day 26th day 26th day Date 0th day 7th day 21st day 21st day
Group name common common comparison experimental Group name common common comparison experimental
n 18 18 10 8 n 18 18 10 8
Search time for security chamber, s Time spent near the security chamber, s Number of approaches to the security chamber, conventional units
8.5 ± 1.8 10.5 ± 1.3
10.7 ± 4.0
22.1 ± 2.0 15.2 ± 1.7s 16.6 ± 1.2a
4.5 ± 0.3 4.8 ± 0.7
3.8 ± 0.1
6.2 ± 1.4b 25.4 ± 4.1b c
4.5 ± 0.2c
Note: a - P < 0.05 compared to baseline, b - P < 0.05 compared to the 11th day of Alzheimer's disease, c - P < 0.05 compared to the 26th day of Alzheimer's disease without treatment; the reliability of the differences between the samples was evaluated using ANOVA
Fig. 4. The trajectories ofrats in the Barnes labyrinth after intrahypocampal injection ofAf40 and intranasal injection of liposomal miR-101 (short-term spatial memory testing): a - the original data ofrats; b - 11th day after the introduction of Af40 aggregates; c - the 26th day after the introduction ofAff40; d - after 10 days of intranasal injection of liposomal miR-101, 1 - entrance to the protective chamber, 2 - false holes
When rats were tested for long-term spatial memory on the 7th day of Alzheimer's disease simulation, probable changes were found. The animals forgot the experience gained, a lot of time was spent searching for a protective chamber, sniffing fake holes. When compared with the initial data, the search time for the protective chamber and the number of approaches to it increased (P < 0.05), and the time spent near the chamber decreased (P < 0.05, Table 3).
Subsequently, on the 21st day after the introduction of Aff40 in rats, the violation ofrecognition ofthe familiar location of the protective chamber persisted. This was manifested in the chaotic movements of animals through the maze and ignoring of the opening to the protective chamber
8.5 ± 1.8 16.5 ± 2.0a 9.5 ± 2.1 22.1 ± 2.2 14.0 ± 3.0a 16.4 ± 4.6
Search time for security chamber, s Time spent near the security chamber, s Number of approaches to ttie 45 ± 0.3 3.9 ± 0.3a 3.2 ± 0.1* security chamber,
conventional units_
Note: a - P < 0.05 compared to baseline, b - P < 0.05 compared to the 7th day of Alzheimer's disease; the reliability of the differences between the samples was evaluated using ANOVA.
12.0 ± 2.5 21.5 ± 7.6
3.5 ± 0.4
Fig. 5. The trajectories of rats ofboth groups in the Barnes labyrinth after intrahypocampal injection of Aff40 and intranasal injection of liposomal miR-101 (long-term spatial memory testing): a - the original data ofrats; b - 7th day after the introduction of Af40; c - 21st day after the introduction of Af40; g - after 10 days of intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101; 1 - entrance to the security chamber, 2 - fake holes
Emotional memory. Studies in rats of emotional memory using the conditioned reflex response of stimulus avoidance found that the quantitative and temporal rates of stable conditioned reflexes to avoid the negative stimulus during intrahypocampal administration of Aff40 aggregates did not change, compared with the rates of conditioned reflexes to. Thus, the latent periods of conditioned reflex responses on the 5th day of Alzheimer's disease simulation were 8.1 ± 0.4 s against the indicators before the introduction of neurotoxin 8.5 ± 1.3 s, the number of conditioned reflex responses was 76.9 ± 10.4% against 72.2 ± 10.1%, respectively. However, the severity of the manifestations of inter-signal reactions changed. There was an increase in the proportion of signaling reactions from 15.4 ± 4.3% to 29.6 ± 5.9%, which reflected a violation of the structure of behaviour in the animals, both at the stage of afferent synthesis and at the stage
c
c
of decision making, which was probably associated with processing of afferent information, the formation of dominant motivation (Fig. 6). In addition, the rats again had reactions to the samples, as an indicator of indecision in the performance of conditioned reflex reactions and violation of mechanisms in the acceptor of the result of the action of Anokhin (1968). A probable increase in the number of unconditional avoidance and grooming reactions (P < 0.05), along with an increase in autonomic reactions (urination, defecation) indicated an increase in anxiety and distress
(Fig. 6).
portion, %
120 100 80 60 40 20 0
a a
A f
f
iii
IV
i
V VI
Fig. 6. Change in intersignal responses in rats after simulation ofAlzhei-
mer's disease and intranasal administration ofliposomal miR-101: □ - initial data of rats, □ - model of Alzheimer's disease (5th day after administration of Ap40 aggregates), Si - after 10 days of treatment with liposomal miR-101 Alzheimer's disease; inter-signal reactions: I - signal reactions, II - sample reactions, III - extra-signal reactions, IV - unconditional avoidance reactions, V - vegetative manifestations, VI - grooming; x ± SD, n = 9; a - P < 0.05 compared with the original data of rats, see Figure 3
Intranasal administration of liposomal miR-101 did not affect the conditioned reflex activity of rats (Fig. 6), but led to the removal of emotional stress in the implementation of conditioned reflex reactions to actively avoid pain stimuli.
Discussion
The cognitive impairments that accompany neurodegenerative age-related diseases are an urgent medical problem today. Animal models are widely used to study the pathogenesis of these diseases, neurobehavioral disorders and the effectiveness of therapy (Zhang et al., 2012; Wahl et al., 2017). Memory impairment is a specific feature of Alzheimer's disease, so the study of associative learning, learning with a change in spatial orientation, working, spatial and emotional memory is an important tool for determining cognitive dysfunction, the establishment of pathogenetic mechanisms and the choice of treatment (Webster et al., 2014; Bengoet-xea et al., 2015; Zarfkar, 2019). Among the behavioral tests for the study of cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease, the most notable are neurobehavioral tests, which are associated with clinical manifestations of memory impairment in humans. The tests used in our study relate to object recognition (working memory), which excludes rewards or penalties (Bengoetxea et al., 2015) and spatial orientation (spatial memory) (Mu-gantseva, 2010; Webster et al., 2014).
Changes in working, spatial and emotional memory in rats caused by intrahypocampal administration of P-amyloid peptide 40 aggregates, are associated with impaired storage and retrieval of information about the experience due to dysfunction of brain structures - hippocampus and anterior neocortex, which agrees with the data (Mugantseva & Podolski, 2009) on the weakening of the interneuronal connections between the hippocampus and the neocortex under the action of aggregated P-amyloid peptide. Earlier Sokolik et al. (2019) showed that the introduction of Ap40 aggregates into the hippocampus leads to an increase in the concentration of toxic endogenous Ap42 by 36.2% in the neocortex and 27.3% in the hippocampus. This increase in the concentration of toxic P-amyloid peptide 42 is characteristic of the development of preclinical and early clinical stages of Alzheimer's disease, when the clinical manifestations of the disease are compensated by brain mechanisms (Purdenko, 2014). Therefore, our model allows us to consider it as adequate for the study of early
manifestations of memory disorders in Alzheimer's disease. It should be noted that the detected memory impairments depend not only on the length oftime the information is stored - short-term or long-term memory, but also on the type of memory. The most demonstrative were violations of the components of short-term and long-term spatial memory, in terms of finding the location of the protective chamber. It has been shown that spatial memory impairment in patients with Alzheimer's disease is associated with impaired interspheric information (Delbeuck et al., 2003). Working memory, as a process of storing information during the experiment, also changed: violations of the differentiation of the shape of objects and decision-making were found.
It is known that the mechanisms of memory are closely related to emotions, their morpho-functional substrate is the limbic-neocortical system of the brain (Vinogradova, 1975; Makarenko, 1980). However, we did not detect emotional memory impairment with the use of the classical conditioned reflex reaction of active avoidance of the pain stimulus at an early stage in the development of experimental Alzheimer's disease. It is clear that the innate forms of protective and adaptive behaviour in this period of the disease are preserved. However, there were manifestations of behavioral components that accompany conditioned reflex responses. These are test reactions, unconditional avoidance reactions and autonomic reactions - urination and defecation, which can be assessed as an increase in anxiety.
Recently, researchers have focused on finding approaches to inhibit amyloidogenesis in Alzheimer's disease by influencing the molecular mechanisms of P-amyloid peptide formation at the posttranscriptional level. In this regard, noteworthy are small non-coding RNA molecules -miRNAs, which are involved in the regulation of gene expression, differentiation, proliferation and apoptosis of cells (Siegel et al., 2011; Olde Loohuis et al., 2012). MicroRNA inhibits gene expression in two ways: by interaction with template RNA or excludes the gene due to epigenetic modulation ofthe genome. It has been shown that miR-101 microRNA is able to inhibit the synthesis of P-amyloid peptide precursor protein (APPP) at the stage of its translation. This leads to the elimination of the accumulation of excess APPP and its processing in the amyloidogenic scenario (Vilardo et al., 2010; Amakiri et al., 2019).
Intranasal administration of miR-101 in liposomal form to rats with a model of early manifestations of Alzheimer's disease led to the improvement of short-term spatial memory, the effectiveness of which was determined by the reproduction of components (skills) of spatial orientation. The improvement in working memory was manifested in the restoration of the level of research activity of rats and the differentiation of familiar and unfamiliar objects. Apparently, the positive effects of miR-101 when introduced into the body in liposomal form are due to their intranasal delivery to the target brain structures of the hippocampus and neocortex and the regulatory effect on the processes of amyloidosis.
Conclusion
Intranasal administration of miR-101 in liposomal form to late adult rats with an experimental model of Alzheimer's disease, resulting from the introduction of P-amyloid peptide 40 aggregates into the hippocampus, improves working and spatial memory: restores the level of research activity objects; increases the efficiency of spatial memory due to the reproduction of spatial orientation skills; improves the emotional state of rats in the implementation of conditioned reflex reactions of active avoidance of painful stimuli.
References
Abdurasulova, I. N., Ekimova, I. V., & Chernyshjov, M.V. (2019). Narusheniye kognitivnykh funktsiy u krys Vistar v modeli doklinicheskoy stadii bolezni Parkinsona [Cognitive impairment in Wistar rats in a model of the preclinical stage of Parkinson's disease]. Zhurnal Vysshey Nervnoy Deyatel'nosti, 69(3), 364-381 (in Russian). Amakiri, N., Kubosumi, A., & Tran, J. (2019). Amyloid beta and MicroRNAs in
Alzheimer's disease. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 430. Anokhin, P. K. (1968). Biologiya i neyrofiziologiya ulovnogo refleksa [Biology and neurophysiology of the catch reflex]. Mediczina, Moscow (in Russian).
a
a
I
II
Antunes, M., & Biala, G. (2012). The novel object recognition memory: Neurobio-logy, test procedure, and its modifications. Cognitive Processing, 13, 93-110.
Attar, A., Liu, T., & Chan, W.-T. C. (2013). A shortened barnes maze protocol reveals memory deficits at 4-months of age in the triple-transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. PLoS One, 8(11), e80355.
Bengoetxea, X., Rodriguez-Perdigon, M., Ramirez, M. J. (2015). Object recognition test for studying cognitive impairments in animal models of Alzheimer's disease. Frontiers in Bioscience, 7, 10-29.
Bevziuk, D. O., Levicheva, N. O., & Sokolik, V. V. (2020). Osoblyvosti poru-shen' pam'yati na ranniy stadiyi rozvytku khvoroby Altsheymera u shchuriv [Features of memory disorders in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease in the rats]. Physiological Journal, 66(1), 68-74 (in Ukrainian).
Bullock, R. (2002). New drugs for Alzheimer's disease and other dementias. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 180(2), 135-139.
Buresh, Y., Petran, M, & Zakhar, I. (1962). Elektro-fiziologicheskiye metody issledovaniya [Electro-physiological research methods]. Izdatel'stvo Inost-rannoj Literatury', Moscow (in Russian).
Delbeuck, X., Van der Linden, M., & Collette, F. (2003). Alzheimer's disease as a disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychology Review, 13, 79-92.
Eisenstein, M. (2019). Pharma's roller-coaster relationship with RNA therapies. Nature, 574, 10-16.
Francis, P. T., Ramirez, M. J., & Lai, M. K. (2010). Neurochemical basis for symptomatic treatment of Alzheimer's disease. Neuropharmacology, 59, 221-229.
Grigorchuk, O. S. (2013). Rol' neyronov dorsal'nogo gippokampa v mekhaniz-makh formirovaniya razlichnykh emotsional'no-motivatsionnykh sostoyaniy u krys: Eifekty peptida del'ta-sna [The role of dorsal hippocampal neurons in the mechanisms of formation of various emotional and motivational states in rats: Effects of the delta sleep peptide]. Moscow (in Russian).
Guljaev, S. M., Taraskin, V. V., & Radnaeva, L. D. (2017). Antiamnesticheskiy effekt ekstrakta vzdutoplodnika sibirskogo pri skopolamin-indutsirovannoy amnezii [Anti-amnestic effect of the Siberian baffle extract with scopolami-ne-induced amnesia]. Obzory po Klinicheskoy Farmakologii i Lekarstven-noy Terapii, 15(4), 53-54 (in Russian).
Huijgen, J., & Samson, S. (2015). The hippocampus: A central node in a large-scale brain network for memory. Revue Neurologiqu (Paris), 171(3), 204-216.
Insel, N., & Takehara-Nishiuchi, K. (2013). The cortical structure of consolidated memory: A hypothesis on the role of the cingulate-entorhinal cortical connection. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 106(7), 343-350.
Makarenko, U. A. (1980). Sistemnaya organizatsiya emotsional'nogo povedeniya [Systemic organization of emotional behavior]. Mediczina, Moscow (in Russian).
Mugantseva, E. A. (2010). Issledovaniye prostranstvennoy pamyati i spektral'no-korrelyatsionnykh kharakteristik EEG gippokampa i frontal'noy kory na modeli bolezni Altsgeymera [The study of spatial memory and spectral-correlation characteristics of the EEG of the hippocampus and frontal cortex on a model of Alzheimer's disease]. Pushchino (in Russian).
Mugantseva, E. A., & Podolski, I. Y. (2009). Animal model of Alzheimer's disease: Characteristics of EEG and memory. Central European Journal of Biology, 4(4), 507-514.
Mukhin, V. (2013). Patogeneticheskie mehanizmyi disfunktsii bazalnoy holinergi-cheskoy sistemyi pri bolezni Altsgeymera [Pathogenesis of the basal forebrain cholinergic dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease]. Rossijskii Fiziologicheskij Zhurnal imeni I. M. Sechenova, 99(7), 793-804 (in Russian).
Olde Loohuis, N. F., Kos, A., & Martens, G. J. (2012). MicroRNA networks direct neuronal development and plasticity. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 69, 89-102.
Puidenko, T. I. (2014). Problema kognityvnykh rozladiv u nevrologichnij praktyci (og-lyad literatury) [The problem of cognitive impairment in neurological practice (reference review)]. Bukovinian Medical Herald, 18(4), 216-221 (in Ukrainian).
Raven, F., Ward, J. F., & Zoltowska, K. M. (2017). Soluble Gamma-secretase modulators attenuate Alzheimer's p-amyloid pathology and induce conformational changes in presenilin 1. EBioMedicine, 24, 93-101.
Shaik, M. M., Tamargo, I. A., & Abubakar, M. B. (2018). The role of microRNAs in Alzheimer's disease and their thera-peutic potentials. Genes (Basel), 9(4), 174.
Shulga S. M. (2014). Obtaining and characteristic of curcumin liposomal form. Biotechnologia Acta, 7(5), 55-61.
Siegel, G., Saba, R., & Schratt, G. (2011). MicroRNAs in neurons: Manifold regulatory roles at the synapse. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development, 21, 491-497.
Silachjov, D. H., Shram, S. I., & Shakova F. M. (2008). Formirovanie prostranstven-noj pamjati u krys s ishemicheskim povrezhdeniem prefrontal'noj kory mozga: effekty sinteticheskogo analoga AKTG [The formation of spatial memory in rats with ischemic damage to the prefrontal cortex: effects of a synthetic analogue of ACTH]. Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnai Deiatelnosti I. P. Pavlova, 58(4), 458466 (in Russian).
Sokolik, V. V., Berchenko, O. G., & Shulga, S. M. (2017). Comparative analysis of nasal therapy of curcumin soluble and liposomal forms of rats with model of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease and Parkinsonism, 7(4), 1000357.
Sokolik, V. V., Berchenko, O. H., & Levicheva, N. V. (2019). Anti-amyloidogenic effect of miR-101 in experimental Alzheimer's disease. Biotechnologia Acta, 12(3), 41-49.
Sysoev, U. I. (2019). Mekhanizm dejstvija novogo proizvodnogo etanolamina -bis{2-[(2e)-4-gidroksi-4-oksobut-2-enoiloksi]-N, N-dietiletanamonija} butan-dionata [The mechanism of action of a new derivative of ethanolamine - bis (2-(2e)-hydroxy and oxobut-2-enoxy-n n-diethyl ethanolammonium butanedione]. Eksperimental'naja i Klinicheskaja Farmakologija, 82(4), 3-10 (in Russian).
Tzyy-Nan, H., & Yi-Ping, H. (2014). Novel object recognition for studying memory in mice. Neuroscience, 19, 1-7.
Vilardo, E., Barbato, C., & Ciotti, M. (2010). MicroRNA-101 regulates amyloid precursor protein expression in hippocampal neurons. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 285, 18344-18351.
Vinogradova, O. S. (1975). Gippokamp i pamyat' [Hippocampus and memory]. Nauka, Moscow (in Russian).
Wahl, D., Coogan, S., & Couter, D. (2017). Cognitive and behavioral evaluation of nutritional interventions in rodent models of brain aging and dementia. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 12, 1419-1428.
Wang, W., Kwon, E. J., & Tsai, L.-H. (2012). MicroRNAs in learning, memory, and neurological diseases. Learning Memory, 19, 359-368.
Webster, S. J., Bachstetter, A. D., & Eldik, L. J. (2014). Using mice to model Alzheimer's dementia: An overview ofthe clinical disease and the preclinical behavioral changes in 10 mouse models. Frontiers in Genetics, 5, 88.
Zarifkar, A. H., Zafar, A., & Bagheri, F. V. (2019). Ameliorative effects of different transcranial electrical stimulation paradigms on the novel object recognition task in a rat model of Alzheimer disease. Galen Medical Journal, e1440.
Zhang, R., Xue, G., & Xie, X. (2012). Novel object recognition as a facile behavior test for evaluating drug effects in ApPP/PS1 Alzheimer's disease mouse model. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 31(4), 801-812.